Conditional probability is important for all students!
This article is part of our collection Great Expectations: Probability through Problems.
If that's how it's taught, then yes, it is dry, apparently pointless, and difficult to justify spending time and effort on.
But if conditional probability is about the chance that a positive test for cancer means you actually have cancer - that matters. If it's about understanding why a simplistic approach to probability leads to miscarriages of justice - that matters. These are questions which can have life and death implications.
A psychologist, Gerd Gigerenzer, in Reckoning with Risk: Learning to Live with Uncertainty (Penguin Books, 2002) gives this example:
The chance that even a well-informed person calculates this probability correctly from information presented in this form is not high. That wouldn't matter if it were purely a mathematical problem, but failing to understand information given in this way is at the root of many medical and legal miscalculations. And no - the correct answer is not about 90% - although you could be
forgiven for thinking that it is!
The same problem can be presented in this way:
The answer is that only 10% of women whose mammogram indicates a problem actually have breast cancer - a huge difference.
Gigerenzer did research with doctors and lawyers to see how many of them could interpret the data expressed using percentages and probabilities. His results are alarming.
All of this is by way of saying that it is vital that students - all students, not just the higher achieving groups who will do higher level exams - need to be helped to understand conditional probability.
We believe that our problems provide students with contexts and structure which will enable them to do this:
- The Dog Ate My Homework - this is an introduction to conditional probability, aimed at 12-14 year-olds. A practical experiment using dice and multi-link cubes gives data which can be used to answer questions like 'What is the chance that a student who is not lying about their homework is nevertheless accused?' The practical representation of data using the multi-link cubes helps students to appreciate that this is not the same question as 'What is the chance that a student who is accused is not lying about their homework?'
- Who Is Cheating? - this provides a full introduction to the theoretical probability required by GCSE and similar syllabuses, and is aimed at 14-16+ year-olds. Again, there is a practical experiment using dice and multi-link cubes, providing data for an initial exploration of questions such as 'What is the probability that an athlete who is taking a banned substance tests negative?' and 'What is the probability that an athlete who is not taking the banned substance nevertheless tests positive?'
These scenarios can easily be adapted to others. Teachers on a course in South Africa recently came up with these:
- What is the chance the weather will be sunny tomorrow, if it is raining today?
- What is the chance that a Grade 10 girl will complete her education if she becomes pregnant? How does this compare with a Grade 10 girl who does not become pregnant?
There is a common lesson structure for these problems. Students start by answering questions from their own results, and from the aggregated results of the whole class, using a tree diagram and 2-way table to represent their results and provide a structure to interpret the data. They then use their intuitive understanding of random events (dice and coins) to see what results they would expect, comparing the experimental data with the expected results.
The expected results, also displayed as whole numbers on a tree diagram and 2-way table, provide the data to answer questions which progress from: 'What proportion of people who experience A do we expect to experience B also?' to: 'What is the chance that a person who experiences A experiences B also?'; 'Is this the same as the chance that a person who experiences B also experienced A?'
From this students answer questions of the form: 'Given that a person experiences A, what is the probability that they also experience B?' Reversing the tree diagram enables them to answer 'Given that a person experienced B, what is the probability that they also experienced A?'
Exploring all these forms of questions in this way enables students to investigate data in order to answer worthwhile questions such as 'If a person tests postive for cancer, what is the probability that they are actually suffering from cancer?' or 'If a person has two children who both die apparently from SIDS, what is the probability that this occurred by chance?'
From this, those for whom it is approriate can go onto Bayes' Theorem with a sound understanding of the basis for theory and formula.